La intuicion en la toma de decisiones, en especifico el decidir si alguien es confiable o no, es uno de los temas resbaladizos de la psicologia. El siguiente articulo amplia el cnocimiento sobre que claves visuales y gestos corporales aportan a la desconfianza. Pulsen aqui y veran un video del robot realizando los gestos.
Who’s Trustworthy? A Robot Can Help Teach Us
by Tara Parker-Pope
The New York Times
September 10, 2012
How do we decide whether to trust somebody?
An unusual new study of college students’ interactions with a robot
has shed light on why we intuitively trust some people and distrust
others. While many people assume that behaviors like avoiding eye
contact and fidgeting are signals that a person is being dishonest,
scientists have found that no single gesture or expression consistently
predicts trustworthiness.
But researchers from Northeastern University, the Massachusetts
Institute of Technology and Cornell recently identified four distinct
behaviors that, together, appear to warn our brains that a person can’t
be trusted.
The findings, to be published this month in the journal Psychological
Science, may help explain why we are sometimes quick to like or dislike
a person we have just met. More important, the research could one day
be used to develop computer programs that can rapidly assess behavior in
airports or elsewhere to flag security risks.
In the first experiment, 86 undergraduates from Northeastern were
given five minutes to get to know a fellow student they hadn’t met
before. Half the pairs met face to face; the other half interacted
online by instant message.
Then the students were asked to play a game in which all the players
got four tokens and the chance to win money. A token was worth $1 if a
player kept it for himself or $2 when he gave it to his partner. Players
could win $4 each if both partners kept their tokens, but if they
worked together and traded all four tokens, then each partner could win
$8. But the biggest gain — $12 — came from cheating a partner out of his
tokens and not giving any in return.
Over all, only about 1 in 5 people (22 percent) were completely
trustworthy and cooperative, giving away all their tokens so that each
partner could win $8. Thirteen percent were untrustworthy, keeping all
or most of their tokens. The remaining 65 percent were somewhat
cooperative, giving away two or three tokens but also holding one or two
back for security.
Both groups demonstrated the same level of cooperation. Whether the
students met face to face or online didn’t change their decisions about
how many tokens to give away or keep. But students who met in person
were far better at predicting the trustworthiness of the partner; that
suggested they were relying on visual cues.
“Lack of face-to-face contact didn’t make people more selfish,” said
the study’s lead author, David DeSteno, a professor of psychology at
Northeastern. “But a person’s ability to predict what their partner was
going to do was greater face to face than online. There is something the
mind is picking up that gives you greater accuracy and makes you better
able to identify people who are going to be trustworthy.”
To find out what cues the players were responding to, the researchers
filmed the students’ five-minute conversations before the game started.
They discovered that four specific gestures predicted when a person was
less trustworthy: leaning away from someone; crossing arms in a
blocking fashion; touching, rubbing or grasping hands together; and
touching oneself on the face, abdomen or elsewhere. These cues were not
predictive by themselves; they predicted untrustworthiness only in
combination.
And individuals intuitively picked up on the cues. “The more you saw
someone do this, the more intuition you had that they would be less
trustworthy,” Dr. DeSteno said.
The researchers then conducted an experiment pairing students with a
friendly-faced robot, developed by Cynthia Breazeal, who directs
M.I.T.’s personal robots group.
The setup was basically the same, except the students had a 10-minute
conversation with the robot before they played the game. (The extra
time was needed to help the student get over the “wow” factor of talking
to a robot.) A woman acted as the robot’s voice, but she was unaware of
its movements, which were controlled by two other people. Sometimes the
robot used only typical gestures, like moving a hand or shrugging its
shoulders, but sometimes it mimicked the four cues of distrust: clasping
its hands, crossing its arms, touching its face or leaning away.
Surprisingly, when students saw the robot make the hand and body
gestures associated with distrust, they later made decisions in the
token game that suggested they didn’t trust the robot.
In questionnaires afterward, students in both groups rated the robot
equally likable. But those who had unknowingly witnessed the cues
associated with distrust also rated the robot as less trustworthy,
compared with students exposed to only the conversational gestures.
“It makes no sense to ascribe intentions to a robot,” said an author
of the study, Robert H. Frank, an economics professor at Cornell. “But
it appears we have certain postures and gestures that we interpret in
certain ways. When we see them, whether it’s a robot or a human, we’re
affected by it, because of the pattern it evokes in our brain
responses.” Dr. Frank said the study suggested that there might have
been an evolutionary benefit to cooperation — and, more important, to
the ability to determine who could be trusted.
“One of the interesting big questions in evolution has always been
‘Why do people do the right thing and pass up opportunities for gain
when no one is looking?’ ” he said. “But if you are known to be a
trustworthy person, then you are economically valuable in many
situations, and it’s also valuable to be able to identify who won’t
cheat.
“Life is all about finding people you can trust in different situations.”
No comments:
Post a Comment